Sunday 25 November 2007

Personal productivity

He who sometimes gets obeyed sent a link to a Merlin Mann informal talk on productivity. Jet-lag got me before I made it to the end, but it got me thinking about just how many parts there are to my life, and how they individually get under control.

Sometimes, organising my life is like one of those whack-a-mole games at a fairground. I pin down one part (say work) so it's running smoothly, and immediately another one (say health or friends) pop up and start to unravel. And I'm measurably sane (having been checked for all the possible candidates, I can safely say that I'm sane; just a bit eccentric and occasionally depressed) but I seem to spend much of my life worrying about things I should have already done or ought to be doing without really being able to quantify what exactly they are, and I am so jealous of people who seem to have enough time to not do that. So I got to wondering: how many things do I actually try to organise at once? And the list is...

  • Work: team projects, team admin, own projects, own admin, outside projects & influences etc.
  • Relationships: partner, family, friends, neighbours etc
  • Infrastructure: house maintenance (including finishing turning a 1970s shell into home), housework, car maintenance, home equipment (pc maintenance etc), finances, paperwork
  • Self: mental + physical health, outside image (clothes, not muttering wildly in the streets because I have too much to do etc)
Now I cheated a bit there because a while back (as part of the "stay sane" project), I made a big list of things to do. And ordered it by House, Self, Work and Other people. What was interesting was that I lost the list for a couple of weeks, and when I found it again, I'd done almost all the 'other people' and 'work' tasks, some of the 'house' tasks and almost none of the 'self' tasks. Which got me to thinking 'who is this life for anyway?'.

Sunday 18 November 2007

Toolmaking

One of the things that occasionally fascinates me is how we define ourselves as human by defining other animals as somehow, well, inferior. And one of the areas that we've traditionally done that til recently is definig ourselves as the only toolmakers. Now anyone who's ever looked at an empty snail shell with a hole in the side without going "wow, I never knew they had escape hatches" automatically knows that isn't true. But how untrue? Once more to the literature boys...

Several books and articles (e.g. this 1940s article by Kenneth Oakley) on man the toolmaker take toolmaking as a given; these works move straight to which tools for what purpose, and skip the reasons and methods by which we might have become toolmakers at all. There's a species of early man, homo habilis ("able man" or sometimes "man the toolmaker") named after its toolmaking skills, mainly for an ability, 2.6 million years ago, to create and use a cutting edge on a small stone.

I'll probably come back to people later, as toolmaking appears to be bound up with creativity, definition and even religion. For now, I'll concentrate on the animals, as a useful control group. So, the ones that I know of are:
And lots more behaviours being observed in labs. There are even behavioural ecology groups out there studying this. In the end, this is really a non-post because the arguments are all there, the evidence is known, including that animals will use whatever materials are to hand (or claw or beak) for the tasks that they have, and that well-fed, well-cared-for captive animals have more time and inclination (even with adjustments for other factors) to be creative; the questions really are only of degree. And that's before we talk about elephants and primates painting.

And then there is the use of animals as tools by humans. Which is a whole different subject, and not one for today. Suffice to say that the Mk 7 Dolphin does exist, and even has patents attached.

Thursday 8 November 2007

Defining Creativity

Sometimes I'm going to post some old notes here. This is one of them.

There are many different attempts to define creativity, and work on automating creativity is often inhibited by the au­thors' own definitions. The process of creativity has been divided into several stages. Hadamard's description of Poincare's four stages is used by Boden amongst others: these are

  • preparation ­ define the problem, and attempt to solve it by rational means.
  • incubation ­ generate novel patterns or concepts.
  • inspiration ­ recognise a creative solution.
  • verification ­ rationally compare the solution to the problem.
The preparation and verification stages may not exist be­cause there may not always be a given aim to creative work. Incubation and inspiration are however central to creativity: it always contains a two­ part process of generation of concepts then evaluation of how creative those concepts are.

Defining the problem

The act of finding a problem is usually part of the creative process. Some creativity systems are very focussed on this: for example, in flexible means­end analysis (Jones+Langley) the problem is defined as a current world state and a set of goal conditions. This part of creativity is very closely related to conven­tional learning theory: Thornton (C.Thornton, 1998) has argued that the bias inherent in any recursive learning algo­rithm can be viewed as a form of creativity.
Although I have stated that preparation and verification are not necessarily essential to creativity, they are very impor­tant: perhaps the difference between creativity and random­ness, between human creativity and madness is in its con­nection to a purpose or communication (for example, even in describing art, we speak of its expression).

Generating novel concepts

Three main types of creativity are Boden's (M.Boden, 1990) improbabilist and impossibilist creativity, and a chaotic form of creativity seen in many of the neural­ network based approaches.

  • improbabilist creativity is the construction of new concepts from existing ones, often combining previously­unconnected information to solve a previously­ unseen problem. The lightbulb puzzle (including the information that lightbulbs are hot just after they are on) is an example. Improba­bilist creativity was explored by Koestler (his 'bisoci­ation of matrices' (A.Koestler, 1964)) and discussed by Perkins (DN.Perkins, 1981).
  • impossibilist creativity transforms the space in which a concept can exist. This includes widening the frame of information around a concept being examined, and the removal of assumptions or constraints from the en­vironment in which a concept exists. Jackson Pollock putting his paintings on the floor (removing the as­sumption that paintings need to be vertical) is an ex­ample of this. There are many constraints at play in creation. For in­stance, in the creation of prose, the pattern of stresses in a line is as important as the meanings and rhymes and hidden meanings within a stanza. We work within unspoken rules: creativity can work within these rules (using them as guidelines) or on those rules them­ selves (to create new forms or categories of art or sci­ence).
  • chaotic creativity is where a small mutation of an ex­isting concept is allowed. Beethoven's minute rework­ing of his musical themes until he hit one which was acceptable to him, and Thaler's creativity machine are examples of this.
This seems a reasonable division to work with, although it would be interesting to see whether, when these three forms of creativity are finally modelled, other forms of creative act and process become apparent.

Measuring creativity

Creativity is often confused with the creation of new things. Creativity is not novelty: while generation of concepts is important, it is not effective without their evaluation. Evaluation consists of deciding which solutions are cre­ative, either by clustering them or by using a measure of surprise. To be creative, we need some sense of the differ­ence between what is truly creative and what is just new. We need to have a sense of how to cluster the mutations generated and how to define the boundaries between those clusters: we need a sense of taste or discrimination. Much of this, we can take from work on concept clustering and in­formation fusion, and work on the difference between cre­ative solutions and novel near­miss solutions to a problem, and the change in process that leads to them. As an example, take the humble paperclip. I can bend a pa­perclip into dozens of minutely different and new shapes, but only a few could be seen (without an explaining con­text, which is in itself a creative act) as creative mutations of its original shape.

Comparing the solution to the problem

If the creative process is used to solve a specific prob­lem, then the problem and its potential solutions need to be matched. Again, this process is closely related to the process of assessing the output from conventional learning algorithms.